Edge in the News: 2009

THE WALKMAN OF GENETIC ENGINEERING: THE MOVE FROM SCIENCE TO A NEW WORLD OF PRODUCTS
SUEDDEUTSCHE ZEITUNG [8.12.09]

...Genetic engineering is now at a point where computer science was around the mid-eighties. The early PCs were limited as to purpose and network. In two and a half decades, the computer has led us into a digial world in which every aspect of lives has been affected. According to Moore's Law, the performance of computers doubles every 18 months. Genetic engineering is following a similar growth. On the last weekend in July, Craig Venter and George Church met in Los Angeles to lead a seminar on synthetic genetic engineering for John Brockman's science forum Edge.org.

Genetic engineering under Church has been following the grwoth of computer science growing by a factor of tenfold per year. After all, the cost of sequencing a genome dropped from three billion dollars in 2000 to around $50 000 dollars as Stanford University's Dr. Steven Quake genomics engineer announced this week. 17 commercial companies already offer similar services. In June, a "Consumer Genetics" exhibition was held in Boston for the first time. The Vice President of Knome, Ari Kiirikki, assumes that the cost of sequencing a genome in the next ten years will fall to less than $1,000. In support for this development, the X-Prize Foundation has put up a prize of ten million dollars for the sequencing of 100 full genomes within ten days for the cost of less than $10,000 dollars per genome sequenced. 

It is now up to the companies themselves to provide an ethical and legal standing to commercial genetic engineering. The States of New York and California have already made the sale of genetic tests subject to a prescription. This is however only a first step is to adjust a new a new commercialized science which is about to cause enormous changes similar to those brought about be computer science. Medical benefits are likely to be enormous. Who knows about dangers in its genetic make-up, can preventive measures meet. The potential for abuse is however likewise given. Health insurances and employers could discriminate against with the DNS information humans. Above all however our self-understanding will change. Which could change, if synthetic genetic engineering becomes a mass market, is not yet foreseeable. For example, Craig Venter is working on synthetic biofuels. If successful, such a development would re-align technology, economics and politics in a fundamental way. Of one thing we can already be certain. The question of whether genetic engineering will becomes available for all is no longer on the table. It has already happened.

Read the full article →

THE NEW YORK REVIEW OF BOOKS [8.12.09]

dyson_1-081309.jpg

Science Musuem, London/Bridgeman Art Library

The first balloon crossing of the English Channel, January 7, 1785; detail from an oil painting by E.W. Cocks, circa 1840

The Age of Wonder means the period of sixty years between 1770 and 1830, commonly called the Romantic Age. It is most clearly defined as an age of poetry. As every English schoolchild of my generation learned, the Romantic Age had three major poets, Blake and Wordsworth and Coleridge, at the beginning, and three more major poets, Shelley and Keats and Byron, at the end. In literary style it is sharply different from the Classical Age before it (Dryden and Pope) and the Victorian Age after it (Tennyson and Browning). Looking at nature, Blake saw a vision of wildness:

Tyger, tyger, burning bright,
In the forests of the night;
What immortal hand or eye,
Could frame thy fearful symmetry?

Byron saw a vision of darkness:

The bright sun was extinguish’d, and the stars
Did wander darkling in the eternal space,
Rayless, and pathless, and the icy earth
Swung blind and blackening in the moonless air….

During the same period there were great Romantic poets in other countries, Goethe and Schiller in Germany and Pushkin in Russia, but Richard Holmes writes only about the local scene in England.

Holmes is well known as a biographer. He has published biographies of Coleridge and Shelley and other literary heroes. But this book is primarily concerned with scientists rather than with poets. The central figures in the story are the botanist Joseph Banks, the chemists Humphry Davy and Michael Faraday, the astronomers William Herschel and his sister Caroline and son John, the medical doctors Erasmus Darwin and William Lawrence, and the explorers James Cook and Mungo Park. The scientists of that age were as Romantic as the poets. The scientific discoveries were as unexpected and intoxicating as the poems. Many of the poets were intensely interested in science, and many of the scientists in poetry.

The scientists and the poets belonged to a single culture and were in many cases personal friends. Erasmus Darwin, the grandfather of Charles Darwin and progenitor of many of Charles’s ideas, published his speculations about evolution in a book-length poem, The Botanic Garden, in 1791. Humphry Davy wrote poetry all his life and published much of it. Davy was a close friend of Coleridge, Shelley a close friend of Lawrence. The boundless prodigality of nature inspired scientists and poets with the same feelings of wonder. The Age of Wonder is popular history at its best, racy, readable, and well documented. The subtitle, “How the Romantic Generation Discovered the Beauty and Terror of Science,” accurately describes what happened.

Holmes presents the drama in ten scenes, each dominated by one or two of the leading characters. The first scene belongs to Joseph Banks, who sailed with Captain James Cook on the ship Endeavour. This was Cook’s first voyage around the world. One of the purposes of the expedition was to observe the transit of Venus across the disc of the sun on June 3, 1769, from the island of Tahiti in the South Pacific. The tracking of the transit from the Southern Hemisphere, in combination with similar observations made from Europe, would give astronomers more accurate knowledge of the distance of the earth from the sun. Banks was officially chief botanist of the expedition, but he quickly became more interested in the human inhabitants of the island than in the plants. The ship stayed for three months at Tahiti, and he spent most of the time, including the nights, ashore. During the nights he was not observing plants.

A wealthy young man accustomed to aristocratic privileges in England, Banks quickly made friends with the Tahitian queen Oborea, who assigned one of her personal servants, Otheothea, to take care of him. With the help of Otheothea and other good friends, he acquired some fluency in the Tahitian language and customs. His journal contains a Tahitian vocabulary and detailed descriptions of the people he came to know. When the time came to set up the astronomical instruments and observe the transit of Venus, he took the trouble to explain to his Tahitian friends what was happening. “To them we shewd the planet upon the sun and made them understand that we came on purpose to see it.”

During the long months at sea after leaving Tahiti, Banks rewrote his journal entries into a formal narrative, “On the Manners and Customs of the South Sea Islands,” one of the founding documents of the science of anthropology. In a less formal essay written after his return to England, he wrote:

In the Island of Otaheite where Love is the Chief Occupation, the favourite, nay almost the Sole Luxury of the Inhabitants, both the bodies and souls of the women are modeld in the utmost perfection for that soft science.

The Tahiti that he describes was truly an earthly paradise, not yet ravaged by European greed and European diseases, twenty years before the visit of William Bligh and the Bounty mutineers, sixty-six years before the visit of Charles Darwin and the Beagle.

After exploring the South Seas, Cook sailed down the eastern coast of Australia and landed at Botany Bay. Banks failed to establish social contacts with the Australian aborigines and returned to his role as botanist, bringing back to England a treasure trove of exotic plants, many of them today carrying his name. After he returned to England, he found that he and Captain Cook had become public heroes. He was invited to meet King George III, who was then young and of sound mind and shared his passion for botany. He remained a lifelong friend of the King, who actively supported his creation of the national botanic garden at Kew.

Banks became president of the Royal Society in 1778 and held that office for forty-two years, officially presiding over British science for more than half of the Age of Wonder. He presided with a light hand and did not attempt to turn the Royal Society into a professional organization like the academies of science in Paris and Berlin. He believed that science was best done by amateurs like himself. If some financial support was needed for people without private means, it could best be provided by aristocratic patrons.

One of those for whom Banks found support was William Herschel, the greatest astronomer of the age. Herschel was a native Hanoverian, and was conscripted at the age of seventeen to fight for Hanover in the Seven Years’ War against the French. After surviving a battle that the Hanoverians lost, he escaped to England to begin a new life as a professional musician. Starting as a penniless refugee, he rose rapidly in the English musical world. By his late twenties he was director of the orchestra in the Pump Room at Bath, the health resort where people of wealth congregated to take the waters and listen to concerts. He stayed at Bath for sixteen years, running the musical life of the city by day and scanning the sky at night. As an astronomer he was a complete amateur, unpaid and self-taught.

At the beginning, when Herschel began observing the heavenly bodies, he believed that they were inhabited by intelligent aliens. The round objects that he saw on the moon were cities that the aliens had built. He continued throughout his life to publish wild speculations, many of which turned out later to be correct. He had two great advantages as an observer. First, he built his own instruments, and with his musician’s hands made exquisitely figured mirrors that gave sharper images than any other telescopes then existing. Second, he brought his younger sister Caroline over from Hanover to be his assistant, and she became an expert observer with many independent discoveries to her credit. His life as an amateur ended in 1781 when with Caroline’s help he discovered the planet Uranus.

As soon as Banks heard of the discovery, he invited Herschel to dinner, introduced him to the King, and arranged for him to be appointed the King’s personal astronomer with a salary of £200 a year, later supplemented by a separate salary of £50 a year for Caroline. Herschel’s musical career was over, and he spent the rest of his life as a professional astronomer. He obtained royal funding to build bigger telescopes, and embarked on a systematic survey of every star and nebulous object in the sky, pushing his search outward to include objects fainter and more distant than anyone else had seen.

Herschel understood that when he looked at remote objects he was looking not only into deep space but into deep time. He correctly identified many of the nebulous objects as external galaxies like our own Milky Way, and calculated that he was seeing them as they existed at least two million years in the past. He showed that the universe was not only immensely large but immensely old. He published papers that moved away from the old Aristotelian view of the heavens as a static domain of perpetual peace and harmony, and toward the modern view of the universe as a dynamically evolving system. He wrote of “a gradual dissolution of the Milky Way” that would provide “a kind of chronometer that may be used to measure the time of its past and future existence.” This idea of a galactic chronometer was the beginning of the new science of cosmology.

As Holmes’s account suggests, all the leading scientists of the Romantic Age, like Banks and Herschel, started their lives as brilliant, unconventional, credulous, and adventurous amateurs. They blundered into science or literature in pursuit of ideas that were often absurd. They became sober professionals only after they had achieved success. Another example was Humphry Davy, who originally intended to be a physician and worked, as part of his medical training, as an assistant at the Pneumatic Institution in Bristol. The Pneumatic Institution was a clinic where patients were treated for ailments of all kinds by inhaling gases. Among the gases available for inhaling was nitrous oxide. Davy experimented enthusiastically with nitrous oxide, using himself and his friends, including Coleridge, as subjects. After one of these sessions, he wrote:

I have felt a more high degree of pleasure from breathing nitrous oxide than I ever felt from any cause whatever—a thrilling all over me most exquisitely pleasurable, I said to myself I was born to benefit the world by my great talents.

Davy was so popular in Bristol that he was invited at the age of twenty-three to become assistant lecturer in chemistry at the Royal Institution in London. The Royal Institution was a newly founded venture that provided “regular courses of philosophical lectures and experiments” for fashionable London audiences. For the preparation of experimental demonstrations to astound and educate the public, the lecturer was provided with a laboratory where he could also do original research.

Davy promptly switched his research activities from physiology to chemistry. He became the first electrochemist, using a huge electric battery to decompose chemical compounds, and discovered the elements sodium and potassium. Later he invented the Davy safety lamp, which made it possible for coal miners to work underground without killing themselves in methane explosions. The lamp made him even more famous. Coleridge invited him to move north and establish a chemical laboratory in the Lake District where Coleridge and Wordsworth lived. Coleridge wrote to him, “I shall attack Chemistry like a Shark.” Davy wisely stayed in London, where he succeeded Banks as president of the Royal Society and chief panjandrum of British science. The poet Byron gave him a couple of lines in his poem Don Juan :

SPIEGEL ONLINE [8.12.09]

"Lächerlichkeit des Gefallenwollens"

Die "NZZ" fragt: Wie wird Google E-Books "verkaufen"? Und wem "gehören" sie dann eigentlich? Und "Die Zeit" bringt eine Exklusivgeschichte: Vor 250 Jahren erfanden Goethe und Schiller die Freundschaft.

 

Neue Zürcher Zeitung, 13.08.2009

Joachim Güntner graut es vor Googles Plänen für sein Feature "Edition", einer ArtCloud-Bibliothek, über die Leser Zugang zu E-Books erhalten, die auf einem Google-Server liegen. Selbst besitzen wird man sie nicht. "Sollten wird damit hadern, dass die Herren dieses neuen Wolkenkuckucksheims theoretisch überjeden Griff ins virtuelle Bibliotheksregal informiert sind? Dass sie sogar ein ungefähres Bild unseres Lesetempos erhalten? Das lässt sich ja doch alles ohne weiteres dokumentieren, wird aber die um ihre Intimität unbekümmerteGeneration Facebook nicht weiter stören." Weitere Artikel: Stanislaus von Moos, emeritierter Professor für Kunstgeschichte, lobt den künstlerischen und didaktischen Anspruch der Berliner Museumsinsel und fragt sich, warum das im Zürcher Landesmuseum nicht denkbar ist. Susanne Ostwald informiert auf der Kinoseite über den neuen Almodovarfilm "Zerrissene Bilder", seiner Liebe zum Kino und zu Penelope Cruz. Christoph Egger stellt den Film "Tödliches Kommando" vonKathryn Bigelow vor. 

Besprochen werden August Strindberg Reportage "Unter französischen Bauern" , der Roman von Felix Philipp Ingold "Gegengabe - aus kritischen, poetischen und privaten Feldern" (mehr dazu in der Bücherschau des Tages ab 14 Uhr).

Die Tageszeitung, 13.08.2009

Der britische Künstler und Filmemacher Steve McQueen erklärt im Interview, warum es in seinem Spielfilmdebüt "Hunger" über den Hungerstreik von IRA-Häftlingen Anfang der 80er-Jahre eine 22-minütige Gesprächsszene gibt: "Wenn Gewalt ins Extrem getrieben wird, dann wird auch Sprache ins Extrem getrieben. Die Häftlinge waren ausgesprochen eloquent, weil sie die Sprache voll ausschöpften, um ihre Realität erfassen zu können. Wenn sie mit dem Sprechen fertig sind, geschieht die Gewalt, und wenn die Gewalt an ihr Ende kommt, beginnt wieder das Sprechen. Das wird im Film evident. Ich wollte, dass die Szene wie das Wimbledon-Finale von Connors und McEnroe würde."

Besprochen werden Sion Sonos Spielfilm "Love Exposure" über einen Liebe suchenden Teenager, das Spielfilmdebüt "Ein Augenblick Freiheit" von Arash T. Riahi, das von iranischen Flüchtlingen in Ankara erzählt, Henry SelicksAnimationsfilm "Coraline" sowie der Roman "Brüder" von Yu Hua über das China des 20. Jahrhunderts (mehr dazu in unserer Bücherschau des Tages ab 14 Uhr).

In tazzwei räsoniert Jan Feddersen über die Geburt der zuweilen durchaus nervenden alternativen Bewegung in Woodstock. Und Katia Meade informiertüber eine schwarze Romanfigur, die auf dem Buchcover von Justine Larbalestiers Thriller "Liar" ein weißes Gesicht bekam, weshalb der amerikanische Verlag nun zurückrudern muss.

Hier Tom.

Aus den Blogs, 13.08.2009

Thomas Rohde hat sich für Bewegte Lettern die Mühe gemacht, einen VortragLawrence Lessigs zum Google Book Settlement teilweise zu transskribieren. Lessig ist kritisch: "We're building into this not so much a digital library but adigital bookstore."

Netzpolitik hat zu ironischen Variationen eines Wahlkampfplakats von Wolfgang Schäuble aufgerufen. Dagegen wollte die Fotografin, die Schäuble für das Plakat portraitierte, klagen - aber sie verzichtet nun doch. Sie hätte auch nicht viel Aussicht auf Erfolg gehabt, meint der Rechtsanwalt Udo Vetter in seinemLawblog: "Zunächst hat die Fotografin in einem Punkt recht. Das Urheberrechtfür die Fotos liegt bei ihr. Schon deswegen, weil man das Urheberrecht nicht übertragen kann. Was man als Inhaber des Urheberrechts allerdings übertragen kann, sind die Nutzungsrechte. Wie weit diese im vorliegenden Fall jedenfalls gehen, lässt sich auf der Homepage der CDU nachlesen, wo es auch die Plakate zum Download gibt: 'Alle Bilder auf www.bilder.cdu.de können für redaktionelle Zwecke unter Angabe des Bildnachweises (Foto: www.bilder.cdu.de) sowie des Fotografen (soweit genannt) kostenlos verwendet werden.'"

(Via BoingBoing) Gehirnpraline, gefälligst?

Frankfurter Rundschau, 13.08.2009

Pitt von Bebenburg sieht mit der Recherche "Die Vier" des FAS-Politikchefs Volker Zastrow auch die Berichterstattung der FAZ zu Andrea Ypsilantis Widersachernrevidiert: Zumindest zwei von ihnen haben aus gar nicht so hehren Gewissensgründen agiert, wie es die FAZ mit ihrer damaligen Schlagzeile "Die fantastsischen Vier" wollte. In Times mager empfiehlt Ina Hartwig allerdings auch in diesem Fall, mehr Proust zu lesen: "Dann nämlich wäre Zastrow die herzergreifende Figur des Baron de Charlus vertraut, der sich im Kampf gegen das unaufhaltsame Werk der Zeit das Haar färbt und die Wangen pudert und dabei eine so traurige Würde ausstrahlt, dass die Lächerlichkeit des Gefallenwollens im Nu verpufft."

Weiteres: Ulrich Kurth berichtet vom Klavierfestival in La Roque d'Antheron. Auf der Medienseite beschwert sich Daniel Bouhs über die Praxis der Hypo Real Estate, die bei ihrer Hauptversammlung keine journalistischen Aufzeichnungen zuzulassen.

Besprochen werden Steve McQueens Film "Hunger" über den Hungerstreik der IRA, Kathryn Bigelows Irakfilm "Tödliches Kommando" und der 3D-Kinderfilm"Coraline".

Die Zeit, 13.08.2009

Ein in aller Bandbreite ausgeschöpftes Sommerthema ist die Freundschaft, die vor 250 Jahren von Goethe und Schiller erfunden wurde (so ähnlich meldet die Seite eins). Der Philosoph und Autor Rüdiger Safranski spricht über Goethe und Schiller, um deren Verhältnis zueinander es in seinem neuem Buch geht: "Freiheit ist ja immer durch Ressentiment und durch Neid bedroht. Du kannst Ressentiment nur überwinden, indem du offensiv das, was bewundernswert ist, bewunderst. Schiller entschließt sich zur Freundschaft mit Goethe, sie ist ihm nicht in den Schoß gefallen."

Außerdem schreibt Elisabeth von Thadden über die Brüderlichkeit unter Frauen; Adam Soboczynski erklärt, warum Kleist von Goethe und Schiller außen vor gelassen wurde; Peter Kümmel dechiffriert die Codes der Freundschaft in Blog, E-Mail und SMS; Eberhard Straub untersucht die besondere Begabung derDeutschen zur Freundschaft; Ijoma Mangold bekennt sich dazu, auch unterKollegen Freunde zu haben.

Weitere Artikel: Den Typus des Kino-Soldaten findet Katja Nicodemus in Kathryn Bigelows Film "Tödliches Kommando" und anderen Irakkriegsfilmen: "Männer, die ihrem Job mit der Nüchternheit von Postboten oder Automechanikern, nur eben in kugelsicheren Westen nachgehen." Carolin Emcke besucht den israelischenSchriftsteller David Grossman. Der New Yorker Theatermann Tuvia Tenenboom hat im Auftrag der Zeit Bayreuth besucht und kann Woody Allen beruhigen: Wagner hören hat ihn nicht dazu animiert, Polen zu überfallen. Der Kunsthistoriker Wolfgang Ullrich huldigt der Kunst des Bad Painting. Detlef Kuhlbrodt feiert Sion Sonos Film "Love Exposure" als japanisch, katholisch und albern. Zu den Mitarbeitern der Woche kürt Ulrich Stock die Musiker der Band Herman Dune.

Auf den Literaturseiten werden unter anderem Yu Huas Roman "Brüder" undPeter Stamms "Sieben Jahre" besprochen (mehr ab 14 Uhr in unsererBücherschau des Tages).

Die Welt, 13.08.2009

Rolf Schneider wundert sich aus Anlass des heutigen Jahrestages, warum noch nie eine Literaturgeschichte der Mauer geschrieben wurde: "Merkwürdiges träte zutage: Unmittelbar nach dem 13. August 1961 richteten die damaligen West-Berliner Autoren Günter Grass und Wolfdietrich Schnurre an den DDR-Schriftstellerverband einen Offenen Brief mit der Aufforderung, 'das Unrecht vom 13. August beim Namen zu nennen ... Wer schweigt, wird schuldig.' Die Antwort verfasste Stephan Hermlin, ein als eher liberal geltender DDR-Autor: 'Von welchem Unrecht sprechen Sie? ... Was am 13. August erfolgte, war ein logischer Schritt in einer Entwicklung, die nicht von dieser Seite der Stadt eingeleitet wurde.'"

Weitere Artikel: Wolfgang Kraushaar fragt, ob es eine Mitschuld der Studentenbewegung an Theodor W. Adornos Tod vor vierzig Jahren gegeben hat. Matthias Heine liest einen Band der "Mitteilungen des deutschen Germanistenverbandes", der zu einem recht optimistischen Befund über die Vitalität der deutschen Sprache kommt. Dankwart Guratzsch wundert sich, dass die Dresdner nach der umstrittenen und teuren Waldschlösschenbrücke nun auch noch eine ganze Menge Tunnel bauen wollen. Daniel Müller unterhält sich mit dem Popsänger Jan Delay über sein neustes Album. Elmar Jung porträtiert die rechtspopulistische dänische Politikerin Pia Kjaersgaard, die chauvinistische Töne in der Kulturpolitik anschlägt. Manuel Brug besucht das Rossini-Festival in Pesaro.

Besprochen werden Filme, darunter ein Biopic über Coco Chanel mit Audrey Tautou.

Weitere Zeitungen, 13.08.2009

Das Wachstum der (englischen) Wikipedia verlangsamt sich, meldet derGuardian: "While the encyclopedia is still growing overall, the number of articles being added has reduced from an average of 2,200 a day in July 2007 to around1,300 today."

Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung, 13.08.2009

Richard Kämmerlings kann keine Krebsbücher mehr lesen; auch das demnächst erscheinende von Georg Diez über den "Tod seiner Mutter" gefällt ihm aus prinzipiellen Gründen nicht: Keiner, findet Kämmerlings, entkommt bei dem Thema dem Boulevard. Hannes Hintermeier berichtet über die Bestrebungenseparatistischer Franken, die nach Bayern gebrachte Beutekunst "an die fränkischen Ursprungsorte" zurückzubringen. In der Glosse informiert Dirk Schümer über italienische Ideen, der Nationalhymne regionale Hymnenbeizugesellen. Christian Wilhagen kann sich sehr für die Bachwoche (Website) in der Stadt Ansbach begeistern, obwohl der Meister selbst zeitlebens nie vor Ort war. Andreas Kilb weiß, was sich in der Bibliothek des Bremer Historikers Gerhard Knoll befindet, die die Stiftung Preußische Schlösser und Gärten soeben angekauft hat. Der Jurist Christoph Möllers erklärt, worin das Problem des Vertrags zwischen dem Land Brandenburg und der jüdischen Gemeinde liegt, den das Bundesverfassungsgericht nun für ungültig erklärt hat. Auf der Kinoseite schreibt Rüdiger Suchsland über die Anime-Reihe (Website) beim Filmfestival von Locarno.

Besprochen werden die "John Wesley"-Ausstellung in der Fondazione Prada in Venedig, eine große Ausstellung zum Verleger Johann Friedrich Cotta imStadtmuseum Tübingen, Kathryn Bigelows Irakkriegs-Film "Tödliches Kommando"und Bücher, darunter Peter-Andre Alts Studie "Kafka und der Film" (mehr dazu in der Bücherschau des Tages ab 14 Uhr).

Süddeutsche Zeitung, 13.08.2009

Von aktuellen Entwicklungen aus der schönen neuen Welt der Genom-Sequenzierung berichtet Andrian Kreye: "Am letzten Juliwochenende trafen sich Craig Venter und George Church in Los Angeles, um für John Brockmans Wissenschaftsforum Edge.org ein Seminar über synthetische Gentechnik zu leiten. Die Gentechnik, so Church, habe die Informatik dabei längst hinter sich gelassen und entwickle sich mit einem Faktor von zehn pro Jahr. Immerhin - der Preis für die Sequenzierung eines Genoms ist von drei Milliarden Dollar im Jahr 2000 auf rund50.000 Dollar gefallen, wie der Ingenieur der Stanford University Dr. Steven Quake diese Woche bekanntgab. 17 kommerzielle Firmen bieten ihre Dienste schon an."

Weitere Artikel: Stephan Speicher erklärt mit historischer Fundierung, dass "Gehaltsexzesse" ökonomisch begründbar sein mögen, deswegen aber noch lange nicht auch gerecht sind. Laura Weissmüller besucht den für den Preis der Neuen Nationalgalerie nominierten Konzeptkünstler Danh Vo in seiner winzigen Kreuzberger Hochhaus-Wohnzelle. Till Briegleb unterhält sich mit Matthias von Hartz, dem künstlerischen Leiter des Internationalen Sommerfestivals auf Kampnagel in Hamburg. Kia Vahland kommentiert den jüngsten, vom Panzerglas abgeschmetterten Teetassenwurf in Richtung Mona Lisa. Vom Filmfestival inLocarno berichtet Fritz Göttler. In der inzwischen SZ-üblichen unkritischen Vorabwerbung bereitet Tobias Kniebe schon mal auf Oskar Roehlers Film über die Entstehung des Films "Jud Süß" vor. Gemeldet wird, dass amerikanische Autoren wie Jonathan Lethem und Michael Chabon von Google Books mehr Privatheit bei der Online-Lektüre fordern (mehr dazu auch in dem Blog BoingBoing. Volker Breidecker gratuliert dem "Subkulturisten" Rolf Schwendtner zum Siebzigsten.

Besprochen werden Alvis Hermanis' in Hallein aufgeführte "Sound of Silence"-Performance, die neu anlaufenden Filme "Tödliches Kommando" von Kathryn Bigelow (Tobias Kniebe staunt über die "Regieamazone in der Menopause"),"Love Exposure" von Sono Sion und "G.I. Joe" von Stephen Sommers sowie Bücher, darunter Peer Pettersons Roman "Ich verfluche den Fluss der Zeit" (mehr dazu in der Bücherschau des Tages ab 14 Uhr).

NEW SCIENTIST [8.6.09]

The Genome Wager

In the spirit of famous scientific wagers by notable scientists, such as Stephen Hawking and Richard Feynman, two leading biologists, Professor Lewis Wolpert and Dr Rupert Sheldrake, have set up a wager on the predictive value of the genome.

The wager will be decided on May 1, 2029, and if the outcome is not obvious, the Royal Society, the world’s most venerable scientific organization, will be asked to adjudicate. The winner will receive a case of fine port, Quinta do Vesuvio, 2005, which should have reached perfect maturity by 2029 and is being stored in the cellars of The Wine Society.

Prof Wolpert bets that the following will happen. Dr Sheldrake bets it will not:

By May 1, 2029, given the genome of a fertilized egg of an animal or plant, we will be able to predict in at least one case all the details of the organism that develops from it, including any abnormalities.

Prof Wolpert and Dr Sheldrake agree that at present, given the genome of an egg, no one can predict the way an embryo will develop. The wager arose from a debate on the nature of life between Wolpert and Sheldrake at the 2009 Cambridge University Science Festival.

[ED. NOTE: This wager began with the replies by Wolpert and Sheldrake to the EdgeQuestion Center 2009.]

THE NEW YORK TIMES — TIERNEY LAB [8.2.09]

There is a growing consensus (at least in Silicon Valley) that the information age is about to give way to the era of synthetic genetics. That was underscored recently when Harvard geneticist George Church and J. Craig Venter — of the race to decode the human genome fame — gave lectures before a small group of scientists, technologists, entrepreneurs, and writers in West Hollywood.

The event, billed as “A Short Course on Synthetic Genomics,” was organized by John Brockman, the literary impresario (and book agent for several New York Times reporters, including this one) who publishes the cybersalon-style website www.edge.org, a forum dedicated to scientists (many of whom are his clients) and their ideas.

In roughly six hours of lectures, both scientists tried to convey how the world will be changed by the ability to routinely read genetic sequences into computing systems and then store, replicate, alter and insert them back into living cells.

The rate at which this technology is now improving puts silicon to shame. Dr. Church noted that between 1970 and 2005 gene sequencing had taken place on a Moore’s Law pace, improving at about 1.5 times per year. Since then it has improved at the rate of an order of magnitude, or ten times annually.

In the process the cost of sequencing the human genome has plunged from $3 billion to $5 thousand and continues to fall. Dr. Church identified 17 companies and one “open source” project all pursuing different technologies to further push down cost and speed up the pace of sequencing.

As a consequence, the structure of the emerging synthetic genetics industry is beginning to mirror that of the semiconductor and computer industries, which are based on modular components and design tools.

The key to the vast growth of the computer industry took place during the 1970s when physicist Carver Mead helped give the industry a standard design approach based on modular components. Now that appears to be happening in the synthetic biology world as well.

For someone who has spent the past three decades writing about computing, Dr. Venter’s talk was eye-opening.

“I view DNA as an analog information system,” he said. “ and I hope to convince you in fact that it is absolutely the software of life.”

http://www.washingtontimes.com/news/2009/jul/19/books-whats-next-dispatches-futu… [7.18.09]

WHAT'S NEXT: DISPATCHES ON THE FUTURE OF SCIENCE
Edited by Max Brockman
Vintage, $15, 256 pages
REVIEWED BY JULIE ROBISON

People's exposure to the world of science is too often limited to watching the Discovery Channel or "reading" National Geographic. But the essence of science is not only what is happening today, but what could happen tomorrow. "What's Next? Dispatches on the Future of Science" is a book of science essays collected and edited by Max Brockman. It boasts that the authors of the 18 original essays that make up this book come from a "new generation of scientists" and are the future of science.

The essays cover a range of topics. In "Will We Decamp for the Northern Rim?," Lawrence C. Smith writes that the world can't escape global warming, regardless of policy changes. Stephon H.S. Alexander discusses dark matter and vacuum energy in "Just What Is Dark Energy." Vanessa Woods and Brian Hare's "Out of Our Minds: How Did Homo sapiens Come Down From the Trees, and Why Did No One Follow?" notes the theory of evolution and its relation to humans is still a work in progress.

In his essay, "Watching Minds Interact," Jason P. Mitchell argues that humans are superior because "natural selection has equipped us with an adaptation more fearsome than teeth or claws: the human brain." He reports how neuroscience has begun to show "how exquisitely sensitive our minds are to the goings-on of the minds around us by suggesting that our brains spontaneously mirror the pattern of activity of other brains in our vicinity." This is important because it means we're social beings; "our brains prefer to be in register with the brains around us."

In tandem, Matthew D. Lieberman's "What Makes Big Ideas Sticky?" explores how minds relate to one another. Mr. Lieberman references great thinkers like Descartes, Thomas Aquinas and Plato and compares Eastern and Western religions, saying that while we would "like to think of our beliefs as stemming from some combination of logical analysis and peer influence," they more likely come from genetic roots. This has been seen recently in multiple studies and Lieberman points to "Baldwin Way, a postdoctoral fellow in my lab at UCLA, [who] has recently come across a key genetic difference between individuals of Eastern and Western descent that differentially affects their brains."

Religion and science are usually subjects that get along as well as water and oil, but it does not stop these scientists from tackling them. Evolution and the big bang theory are both discussed at length from differing perspectives in Sean Carroll's "Our Place in an Unnatural Universe" and in Nick Bostrom's "How to Enhance Human Beings."

"Medical science is difficult," writes Mr. Bostrom. "We know this because, despite our best efforts, it often fails. Yet medicine typically aims merely to fix something that's broken. Human enhancement, by contrast, aims to take a system that's not broken and make it better — in many ways a more ambitious goal." He discusses enhancement to give people more mental energy, to increase DNA repair activity in cells, and improve concentration.

Whether scientists should even be making these types of changes is also called into question; the need to make ethical decisions in science are not uncommon, but Sam Cooke asks in his essay "Memory Enhancement, Memory Erasure: the Future of Our Past" whether scientists should. "Some may argue that it is not the role of scientists to make ethical judgments about the potential impact of their work — that such decisions are the job of the government, or the electorate, who should decide which scientific research is funded by public money and which is not."

Nonetheless, Joshua D. Greene believes there is a science to making moral and ethical decisions. In his essay "Fruit Flies of the Moral Mind," he discusses the "complex interplay between intuitive emotional responses and more effortful cognitive processes" involved with making moral judgments.

"People sometimes ask me why I bother with these bizarre hypothetical dilemmas," says Mr. Greene. "Shouldn't we be studying real moral decision making instead? To me, these dilemmas are like a geneticist's fruit flies. They're manageable enough to play around with in the lab but complex enough to capture something interesting about the wider and wilder world outside." An interesting way to view moral dilemmas; it therefore should not be a surprise that Mr. Greene ends the essay wondering if we can ever "transcend the limitations of our moral instincts." This is especially intriguing after reading Christian Keysers' "Mirror Neurons: Are We Ethical By Nature?" and his remark that the "brain is ethical by design."

Story Continues →

http://latimesblogs.latimes.com/jacketcopy/2009/06/does-language-shape-our-think… [7.17.09]

Checkpoint_languages

An essay on how language influences thought from the pop-science anthology "What's Next: Dispatches on the Future of Science" has been posted on The Edge. Author Lera Boroditsky, an assistant professor of psychology, neuroscience and symbolic systems at Stanford, writes:

Most questions of whether and how language shapes thought start with the simple observation that languages differ from one another. And a lot! Let's take a (very) hypothetical example. Suppose you want to say, "Bush read Chomsky's latest book." Let's focus on just the verb, "read." To say this sentence in English, we have to mark the verb for tense; in this case, we have to pronounce it like "red" and not like "reed." In Indonesian you need not (in fact, you can't) alter the verb to mark tense. In Russian you would have to alter the verb to indicate tense and gender. So if it was Laura Bush who did the reading, you'd use a different form of the verb than if it was George. In Russian you'd also have to include in the verb information about completion. If George read only part of the book, you'd use a different form of the verb than if he'd diligently plowed through the whole thing. In Turkish you'd have to include in the verb how you acquired this information: if you had witnessed this unlikely event with your own two eyes, you'd use one verb form, but if you had simply read or heard about it, or inferred it from something Bush said, you'd use a different verb form.

She brings up experiments and other examples involving use of language and direction, time, color and gender, all of which seem to demonstrate that yes, language shapes how we think.

But my favorite is this example above. Only a linguist -- or perhaps a social scientist -- would put Chomsky in a hypothetical. 

-- Carolyn Kellogg

http://timestranscript.canadaeast.com/opinion/article/727019 [7.12.09]

By Norbert Cunningham

Hello everyone! I've a little science today, but first note that language is a communication tool; it's what allows us to relate our experiences and thoughts, all, of course, as processed by our brains.

And while we don't yet understand how our brains work very well, scientists have lately been making remarkable progress, in part thanks to new technologies such as MRI scanners that allow them to observe healthy (as well as damaged) brains as they work.

Time flies

Now to connect this to language: we've all heard or used the cliché that "time flies."

Anybody over 40 has also likely remarked at how much faster time flies as you get older. It's a common observation many find puzzling: why does summer as a youngster seem "endless," yet pass in the blink of an eye to adults who swear it was only a couple weeks ago that they put the snow shovel away?

Does time speed up in some magical, bizarre way as we age?

Do people in traumatic events like car crashes actually witness time slowing down as they so often report?

The intuitive answer is that these are matters of perception more than reality in which time has been said to flow like a river, sure and steady (only, as Einstein showed, does time actually slow significantly enough to truly notice if you travel at incredibly high speeds beyond anything most of us will ever experience).

That intuition it is our perception has been shown to be correct, so why do we perceive time to speed up as we age? Our everyday language and the millions of people commenting on the fact are not wrong: the perception is real.

Perception

I take the following from an essay titled "Brain Time" by Dr. David M. Eagleman which appears in a book called "What's Next? Dispatches on the Future of Science," edited by Max Brockman.

Dr. Eagleman is a bright young scientist who has undergraduate degrees from Rice University and Oxford University in literature, but who obtained a doctorate in neuroscience from the Baylor College of Medicine 11 years ago. Today he is director of the Baylor College of Medicine's Laboratory for Perception and Action. The lab's long-term goal is to "understand the neural mechanisms of time perception," which in plain English is to figure out how our brains make us think time has slowed or sped up when it hasn't. It was his and his colleagues' work that allowed me to say that our intuitions are correct: people in traumatic situations do perceive time to slow, but a hair-raising experiment shows they have no extra time to react or do anything extra beyond what would normally be possible.

An explanation

We perceive the slow motion because time and memory are "tightly linked," says Dr. Eagleman. In such critical situations a part of our brain called the amygdala kicks into high gear and takes over most of the brain's resources. This forces a secondary memory system to do the processing, a system that can later produce flashbacks of the sort soldiers with post-traumatic stress experience. This backup memory is "stickier" than what our brains usually use to store memories, producing more vivid and clear images in our minds; more detail. And in remembering these, since there are many more images, just like inserting extra images in a movie reel, it makes the event appear to last longer and slows motion down. That much is fairly certain.

Less certain, but strongly suspected by Dr. Eagleman, is that the same or a similar process is what makes time seem to speed up as we age. As we experience ever more in life, familiar patterns recur and the memories our brains store get ever more compressed. Our brain can skip or compress a lot of things we know or have already experienced because we've got the general template from the first time and it need add only new details. As a result, when we draw on our memory, it is much less vivid and detailed, having the effect of cutting some frames out of a film, which seemingly speeds time up. Children, on the other hand, are frequently having first-time experiences, encountering novel things. Their brains store that information in all its detail and richness since it is the first time. Recalling it, even decades later, we remember those "endless summers" and wonder whatever happened to them. This is not "proven" yet, but it sounds logical and fits with what is known about the sensation of time slowing when in a critical situation. It's a good tentative explanation.

Full circle

So it's back to language, which does describe what we perceive well. But time to us is not really a steady flowing river after all. It's relative, according to how our brains stored our memories. And time flies as we age . . . it seems.

The last word

Here is author and wit Douglas Adams:

Time is an illusion. Lunchtime doubly so.

* Lex Talk! is researched and written by Times & Transcript editorial page editor Norbert Cunningham. It appears in this space every Monday.

NEWSWEEK [7.12.09]

About 10 years ago, biology entered betting season. An upstart scientist named J. Craig Venter jolted the genetics establishment by launching his own gene-sequencing outfit, funded by commercial investment, and setting off toward biology's holy grail—the human genome—on his own. It was Venter versus the old guard—old because of where they got their money (governments and trusts) and the sequencing technique they wanted to hold onto. Venter won that race, and not because he got there first. By combining the freedom of academic inquiry and commercial capital, he came up with a new way of doing science so effective that it forced the old institutions to either ramp up or play second fiddle.

With Venter's momentum, biology has continued to surge into new territory, but now he's not alone in pushing the pace. In fact, with his staff of hundreds at the J. Craig Venter Institute, he is looking dangerously like the establishment he raced past almost a decade ago. Another maverick in the stable, Harvard biologist George Church, is a titan in the academic world, tackling the major challenges of genomic-age biology with an ingenuity distinct from Venter's. Both are building on the foundation of DNA sequencing, trying to drive down the cost of decoding individual genomes and—the more radical enterprise—using their digital control of cells and DNA to design new organisms. Between them, Venter and Church direct or influence a major portion of work in both sequencing and synthetic biology, including three different commercial efforts to develop bacteria that could produce the next generation of biofuels.

There's reason to believe that Church has a decent chance of unseating Venter as biology's next wunderkind. The field of genomics is only at the beginning of its growth spurt—sequencing, it turns out, was just phase one. Far from producing answers, the sequenced genome has instead led scientists into a thicket of questions: What exactly do combinations of genetic code produce in an organism over a lifetime? If we can read the script, can we also write it? Leading science out of the genomic wilderness arguably calls for a vision more deeply imaginative than the task of the Human Genome Project, which was clearly framed and, at heart, a code-reading slog. Radical invention—the kind of out-of-left-field inspiration that makes a thinker either brilliant or totally unrealistic—is the strength of Church, as opposed to Venter, who is more of an aggregator, a connector of existing ideas and methods. The script of this new biology is largely unwritten, and just because Venter turned the first page doesn't mean that in the end his vision will prevail. "Sometimes," Church says, "it's best to be second."

The quest for ideas farther afield may be one reason Venter joined the Harvard faculty this spring—his first academic post since 1982. (Venter declined to be interviewed for this article.) He and Church are even members of the same research initiative, called Origins of Life, where they're investigating life in its most basic genetic and molecular forms. Venter's participation is a sign of just how widely applicable the high-concept work of the university could be. More than ever, over the uncarved terrain of the new biology, Venter and Church are blurring the distinction between the academic and the commercial. Steven Shapin, a Harvard historian of science, says that at this point we must "stop categorizing—and just look at what these people are doing." On top of all the daring science, Venter and Church are also conducting a "sociology experiment": "They're making up their own social roles," Shapin says, "making up themselves." All the while, Church insists that he and Venter are "not right on top of each other" but are "part of the same ecosystem," fulfilling different roles. Then again, Shapin points out, "the lion and the wildebeest are in the same ecosystem." The question is, who's the lion?

http://www.washingtontimes.com/news/2009/jul/19/books-whats-next-dispatches-futu… [7.8.09]

WHAT'S NEXT: DISPATCHES ON THE FUTURE OF SCIENCE
Edited by Max Brockman
Vintage, $15, 256 pages
REVIEWED BY JULIE ROBISON

People's exposure to the world of science is too often limited to watching the Discovery Channel or "reading" National Geographic. But the essence of science is not only what is happening today, but what could happen tomorrow. "What's Next? Dispatches on the Future of Science" is a book of science essays collected and edited by Max Brockman. It boasts that the authors of the 18 original essays that make up this book come from a "new generation of scientists" and are the future of science.

The essays cover a range of topics. In "Will We Decamp for the Northern Rim?," Lawrence C. Smith writes that the world can't escape global warming, regardless of policy changes. Stephon H.S. Alexander discusses dark matter and vacuum energy in "Just What Is Dark Energy." Vanessa Woods and Brian Hare's "Out of Our Minds: How Did Homo sapiens Come Down From the Trees, and Why Did No One Follow?" notes the theory of evolution and its relation to humans is still a work in progress.

In his essay, "Watching Minds Interact," Jason P. Mitchell argues that humans are superior because "natural selection has equipped us with an adaptation more fearsome than teeth or claws: the human brain." He reports how neuroscience has begun to show "how exquisitely sensitive our minds are to the goings-on of the minds around us by suggesting that our brains spontaneously mirror the pattern of activity of other brains in our vicinity." This is important because it means we're social beings; "our brains prefer to be in register with the brains around us."

In tandem, Matthew D. Lieberman's "What Makes Big Ideas Sticky?" explores how minds relate to one another. Mr. Lieberman references great thinkers like Descartes, Thomas Aquinas and Plato and compares Eastern and Western religions, saying that while we would "like to think of our beliefs as stemming from some combination of logical analysis and peer influence," they more likely come from genetic roots. This has been seen recently in multiple studies and Lieberman points to "Baldwin Way, a postdoctoral fellow in my lab at UCLA, [who] has recently come across a key genetic difference between individuals of Eastern and Western descent that differentially affects their brains."

Religion and science are usually subjects that get along as well as water and oil, but it does not stop these scientists from tackling them. Evolution and the big bang theory are both discussed at length from differing perspectives in Sean Carroll's "Our Place in an Unnatural Universe" and in Nick Bostrom's "How to Enhance Human Beings."

"Medical science is difficult," writes Mr. Bostrom. "We know this because, despite our best efforts, it often fails. Yet medicine typically aims merely to fix something that's broken. Human enhancement, by contrast, aims to take a system that's not broken and make it better — in many ways a more ambitious goal." He discusses enhancement to give people more mental energy, to increase DNA repair activity in cells, and improve concentration.

Whether scientists should even be making these types of changes is also called into question; the need to make ethical decisions in science are not uncommon, but Sam Cooke asks in his essay "Memory Enhancement, Memory Erasure: the Future of Our Past" whether scientists should. "Some may argue that it is not the role of scientists to make ethical judgments about the potential impact of their work — that such decisions are the job of the government, or the electorate, who should decide which scientific research is funded by public money and which is not."

Nonetheless, Joshua D. Greene believes there is a science to making moral and ethical decisions. In his essay "Fruit Flies of the Moral Mind," he discusses the "complex interplay between intuitive emotional responses and more effortful cognitive processes" involved with making moral judgments.

"People sometimes ask me why I bother with these bizarre hypothetical dilemmas," says Mr. Greene. "Shouldn't we be studying real moral decision making instead? To me, these dilemmas are like a geneticist's fruit flies. They're manageable enough to play around with in the lab but complex enough to capture something interesting about the wider and wilder world outside." An interesting way to view moral dilemmas; it therefore should not be a surprise that Mr. Greene ends the essay wondering if we can ever "transcend the limitations of our moral instincts." This is especially intriguing after reading Christian Keysers' "Mirror Neurons: Are We Ethical By Nature?" and his remark that the "brain is ethical by design."

Story Continues →

BUBBLE TROUBLE
TIMES HIGHER EDUCATION SUPPLEMENT [7.8.09]

The humanities are in the same state financial markets were in before they crashed. Assessing the growing mountain of toxic intellectual debt, Philip Gerrans considers going short on some overvalued research.. ...

...The academic market is also like the financial market in another way. Stocks trade above their value, which leads to bubbles and crashes. Brain- imaging studies, for example, are a current bubble, not because they don't tell us anything about the brain, but because the claims made for them so vastly exceed the information they actually provide. As with a leveraged investment in mortgage bonds hedged by a foreign-exchange credit swap, most customers have no idea how a brain-imaging result is produced and what it is really worth. Those who do - the ones in labs using complicated statistical algorithms to map impossibly messy signals to artificial 3D models of brains - are usually very circumspect about the results. But every week we read in the science pages that brain-imaging studies prove X, where X is what the readers or columnists already believe. Women can't read maps! Men like sex! Childhood trauma affects brain development! There is an Angelina Jolie neuron! The bosses of big labs that employ hundreds of people use these studies, along with artfully placed articles about them, to get funding for future research. In a similar way, directors of mining companies raise funds on the basis of prospecting reports "leaked" to the financial press.

Consider, as an unrivalled piece of hyperbole, this statement from the websiteEdge.org, which aims "to arrive at the edge of the world's knowledge" by seeking out "the most complex and sophisticated minds". It is by Vilayanur S. Ramachandran, a brilliant experimental neuroscientist as well as a master publicist: "The discovery of mirror neurons in the frontal lobes of monkeys, and their potential relevance to human brain evolution ... is the single most important 'unreported' (or at least, unpublicised) story of the decade. I predict that mirror neurons will do for psychology what DNA did for biology: they will provide a unifying framework and help explain a host of mental abilities that have hitherto remained mysterious and inaccessible to experiments."
That's not very likely. Mirror neurons are neurons in the monkey premotor cortex that are active both when a monkey produces an action such as grasping, and when it observes the action. No one yet knows quite why there is an overlap in patterns of neural activity. Ramachandran would like to find out, so he has made his pitch to investors. They know he has done some beautiful experiments and he is a charismatic public performer and Edge.org regular, so we can expect the mirror neuron boom to continue for a while. ...

[ED. NOTE: Philip Gerrans writes: "So we can expect the mirror neuron boom to continue for a while". Is nine years enough time to make this point? See Ramachandran's Edge essay "Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind 'the great leap forward' in human evolution" published on June 1, 2000. —JB]

Read the full article →

http://edge.org/3rd_culture/bios/brockmanm.html [6.30.09]

Written by Sarah Boslaugh 

Each essay is self-contained, making it possible to choose those most relevant to your own interest

 

If your favorite day of the week is Tuesday, because that's when the Science section of The New York Times is published, and your favorite NPR show is Ira Flatow'sScience Times, then you'll love What's Next? Dispatches on the Future of Science, a collection of essays written by young scientists about what they do and how they see the future of their fields. Even if you're not quite that much of a science geek, if you have an interest in the world around you and the process by which scientific research can both explain and mold that world, you'll enjoy this collection edited by Max Brockman. No expertise in any field is required to understand these essays; if you can follow Malcolm Gladwell, you'll have no troubles with What's Next?

Brockman's essayists represent a variety of fields, from physics to paleoanthropology, with a heavy leaning toward the human sciences. This is a good choice from the marketing point of view, since non-scientists tend to be more interested in topics relating to human psychology than, say, the role played by dark energy in accelerating the expansion of the universe, but fans of hard science may feel slighted. That objection aside, this is the perfect collection for people who like to stay up on recent scientific research but haven't the time or expertise to go to the original sources (which, in the case of modern science, usually means articles published in professional journals, which are not generally available to those without access to an academic library).

Each essay is self-contained, making it possible to choose those most relevant to your own interests. And it's a great airplane or beach book because you can read the essays in any order; each is brief enough to be read between the interruptions of gate announcements or children demanding attention. My personal favorite is "What Makes Big Ideas Sticky?" by UCLA psychologist Matthew Lieberman, which argues that ideas which mirror the structure and function of the human brain may seem so obviously true to us that they resist being discarded, even in the face of overwhelming amounts of scientific research demonstrating their lack of merit.

The collection closes with an essay by NASA climatologist Gavin Schmidtentitled "Why hasn't specialization led to the Balkanization of science?" He argues that in contradiction to the stereotype of the scientist as someone who knows more and more about less and less, interdisciplinary research is central to modern science and describes both the factors which lead to greater isolation among fields of research, and those which encourage cooperation and sharing of ideas. Communication of major ideas in nontechnical language is one of the factors which encourages cooperation, and What's Next? represents an important contribution to that effort.

Sarah Boslaugh

256 pages. $14.95 (paperback)

http://latimesblogs.latimes.com/jacketcopy/2009/06/does-language-shape-our-think… [6.17.09]

Checkpoint_languages

An essay on how language influences thought from the pop-science anthology "What's Next: Dispatches on the Future of Science" has been posted on The Edge. Author Lera Boroditsky, an assistant professor of psychology, neuroscience and symbolic systems at Stanford, writes:

Most questions of whether and how language shapes thought start with the simple observation that languages differ from one another. And a lot! Let's take a (very) hypothetical example. Suppose you want to say, "Bush read Chomsky's latest book." Let's focus on just the verb, "read." To say this sentence in English, we have to mark the verb for tense; in this case, we have to pronounce it like "red" and not like "reed." In Indonesian you need not (in fact, you can't) alter the verb to mark tense. In Russian you would have to alter the verb to indicate tense and gender. So if it was Laura Bush who did the reading, you'd use a different form of the verb than if it was George. In Russian you'd also have to include in the verb information about completion. If George read only part of the book, you'd use a different form of the verb than if he'd diligently plowed through the whole thing. In Turkish you'd have to include in the verb how you acquired this information: if you had witnessed this unlikely event with your own two eyes, you'd use one verb form, but if you had simply read or heard about it, or inferred it from something Bush said, you'd use a different verb form.

She brings up experiments and other examples involving use of language and direction, time, color and gender, all of which seem to demonstrate that yes, language shapes how we think.

But my favorite is this example above. Only a linguist -- or perhaps a social scientist -- would put Chomsky in a hypothetical. 

http://www.veryshortlist.com/vsl/daily.cfm/review/1272/Current_cinema/whats-next… [6.16.09]

venn diagram

 

thumbnail

NONFICTION
What's Next? Dispatches
on the Future of Science


Sure, we often hear from the prominent, popular scientists of today: Steven Pinker, Richard Dawkins, E. O. Wilson. But what about the next generation? Who are they, and what are they thinking about? The answers can be found in the engrossing essay collection What’s Next? Dispatches on the Future of Science, which offers a youthful spin on some of the most pressing scientific issues of today—and tomorrow.

Take, for example, Laurence C. Smith's essay on global warming. Instead of rehashing the debate, Smith wonders about the possibility of people migrating to the Northern Rim as temperatures rise and inhospitable environments become more livable. Associate professor of physics Stephon H. S. Alexander tackles dark energy; other pieces address memory, morality, why viruses matter and the inevitability of human extinction. Kinda scary? Yes! Super smart and interesting? Definitely.

SUEDDEUTSCHE ZEITUNG [6.10.09]

Wenn der Kopf im Internet nicht mehr mitkommt: Frank Schirrmachers Buch "Payback" bringt die digitale Debatte zwar auf den neuesten Stand, aber nicht weiter.

Es gibt in der industrialisierten Welt kein Land, in dem die Debatte um den Einfluss des Internets auf die Gesellschaft mit so vielen dogmatischen Verkrustungen und ideologischen Verschärfungen geführt wird, wie in Deutschland. Die digitale Kluft, die sich durch unser Land zieht, verläuft meist entlang der Generationengrenze zwischen "Digital Natives" und "Digital Immigrants", also zwischen jenen, die mit dem Internet aufgewachsen sind, und jenen, die den digitalen Technologien erst als Erwachsene begegneten.

Bild vergrößern

Schirrmachers Stärke ist es, den intellektuellen Wissensdurst mit den Jagdinstinkten eines Boulevardjournalisten zu verbinden. (© Foto: dpa)

SDE.init.trigger( SDE.events.ContentReady.zoomable, edge_ "id": "uid-1-131933-1274122617" );

Dabei ist das Thema längst größer als der knickrige Streit um alte und neue Mediengewohnheiten und Urheberrechtsfragen oder die politische Panikmache vor Amokspielen und Kinderpornos, auf die die digitalen Debatten in Deutschland meist hinauslaufen. Das neue Buch des FAZ-Herausgebers und Feuilletonisten Frank Schirrmacher "Payback" (Blessing Verlag München, 2009, 240 Seiten, 17,95 Euro) erweitert die Debatte nun endlich um kluge Gedanken. Auch wenn der Untertitel "Warum wir im Informationszeitalter gezwungen sind zu tun, was wir nicht tun wollen, und wie wir die Kontrolle über unser Denken zurückgewinnen" zunächst nach der üblichen Mischung aus Kulturpessimismus und Selbsthilfe klingt.

Unterschätzen darf man den Untertitel nicht. Schirrmachers publizistische Stärke ist es, den intellektuellen Wissensdurst eines Universalgelehrten mit den Jagdinstinkten eines Boulevardjournalisten zu verbinden. Das macht den Konkurrenzkampf mit ihm so sportlich und seine Bücher und Debattenanstöße zu Punktlandungen im Zeitgeist. Dass er dabei oft mit Ängsten spielt, wie der Angst vor der Überalterung der Gesellschaft in seinem Bestseller "Das Methusalem-Komplott" oder der Furcht vor der sozialen Entwurzelung in "Minimum", ist seinem Boulevard-Instinkt geschuldet, der solche Ängste schon früh aufspüren und in einen Kontext setzen kann.

Druck der sozialen Verpflichtungen

Auch "Payback" verkauft sich als Begleitbuch zu aktuellen Ängsten. Schirrmacher greift jenes Gefühl der digitalen Überforderung auf, das sich nicht nur in Deutschland, sondern in allen digitalisierten Ländern breitmacht. Denn die Siegeszüge dreier digitaler Technologien haben in den vergangenen beiden Jahren die Grenzen der digitalen Aufnahmebereitschaft ausgereizt.

Da war zunächst das iPhone mit seinen inzwischen rund 20000 "Apps" - Programmen, die aus dem Apple-Handy einen Supercomputer machen. Dann erhöhte die Netzwerkseite Facebook den Druck der sozialen Verpflichtungen im Netz ins Unermessliche. Und schließlich öffnete der Kurznachrichtendienst Twitter die Schleusen für eine Informationsflut, die sich nur noch mit einer Palette von Hilfsprogrammen bewältigen lässt. Längst gibt es in Europa und Amerika unzählige Artikel und Bücher, die diese Überforderung thematisieren.

"Mein Kopf kommt nicht mehr mit", heißt auch das erste Kapitel von "Payback". Da beschreibt Schirrmacher, stellvertretend für viele, seine ganz persönliche kognitive Krise, in die ihn die digitalen Datenmengen gestürzt haben. Wie ein Fluglotse fühle er sich, immer bemüht, einen Zusammenstoß zu vermeiden, immer in Sorge, das Entscheidende übersehen zu haben. Mehr als ein Lassowurf ist dieser Einstieg nicht, denn letztlich führt er über den Identifikationsmoment nur in den ersten der beiden Teile des Buches ein. Und da geht es um mehr.

Lesen Sie auf Seite 2, wie es im zweiten Teil von "Payback" weitergeht.

http://edge.org/3rd_culture/brockman_next09/NS_WhatNext.pdf [6.8.09]

Fallout from the amazing advance in neuroscience dominates this fascinating foray into the future

By Amanda Gefter

FOR PROPHETIC visions of the future, some people turn to horoscopes or fortune tellers. But if you really want to know what the future holds, ask a scientist.

Not just a renowned, seasoned scientist, but a fresh mind, someone who is asking themselves the questions that will define the next generation of scientific thought.

That's precisely what Max Brockman has done in this captivating collection of essays, written by "rising stars in their respective disciplines: those who, in their research, are tackling some of science's toughest questions and raising new ones".

The result is a medley of big ideas on topics ranging from cosmology and climate change, to morality and cognitive enhancement.

The collection is diverse, but one theme resounds: when it comes to the human race, the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. We owe our evolutionary success to our unique modes of social behaviour.

Social species


In their essay "Out of our minds", journalist Vanessa Woods and anthropologist Brian Hare suggest that it wasn't intelligence that led to social behaviour, but rather social behaviour that paved the way for the evolution of human intelligence. "Humans got their smarts only because we got friendlier first," they write.

We are a social species, and we have our brains to thank. As Harvard University neuroscientist Jason Mitchell writes: "The most dramatic innovation introduced with the rollout of our species is not the prowess ofindividual minds, but the ability to harness that power across many individuals."

Language allows us to do this in an unprecedented way — it serves as a vehicle for transferring one's own mental states into another's mind. Lera Boroditsky — a professor of psychology, neuroscience and symbolic systems at Stanford University — has an interesting piece about the ways in which our native language shapes the way we think about such basic categories as space, time and colour. ...

KOREA TIMES [4.9.09]


Translated from English to Korean by Jang Seok-bong and Kim Dae-yeon; Galleon; 563pp., 19,800 won

From global warming to economic crises, things seem to be turning worse. At this time of pessimism prevailing over optimism, the world needs some antidotes to this epidemic of negative views. But what's out there to be positive about?

This is the question that the author asked 160 scholars and scientific thinkers. John Brockman, the founder of Edge, the influential online salon, complied their answers in this book.

Nobel Laureates, Pulitzer Prize winners, Harvard professors and other world class thinkers laid bare their minds about what they're positive about. They are neither blindly nor naively optimistic. Their optimism is based on logical, professional views and insight.

Topics are wide-ranging, from physics and medicine to education and religion or the end of the world. They illustrate diverse sides of the world's future and why they're optimistic about it.

These great thinkers also present tasks that we should tackle to make a better world and this book may help change readers' perceptions of the future of mankind in a more positive way.

-CHO JAE-HYON

THE MAUI NEWS [4.9.09]

While Christians this week observe their holiest week, it might be a good time to contemplate whether their religion is a matter of faith.

There is a faction in the spectrum of Christian believers that persist in seeking to have the government mandate that their beliefs are a science.

Labeled at one time as creationism, it's reincarnated as intelligent design, with proponents insisting that it should be included in public school curriculum alongside Darwin's thesis in "On the Origin of Species."

Numerous court decisions have ruled that efforts to introduce Bible-based curricula on a God-created universe amount to an unconstitutional introduction of state-sponsored religion. Still, advocates continue to pursue mandates to add their "theory" of creation to public school curriculum.

Over the past year, legislative proposals have been offered in Alabama, Florida, Michigan, Missouri, South Carolina and Louisiana seeking to require curriculum that challenge the theory of evolution in the interest of critical analysis and academic freedom.

The persistent effort to cast personal faith in God as a science suggests that proponents of intelligent design don't understand what faith is or lack it. Biblical exhortations to faith occurred when Jesus Christ faced disciples who appeal to him to intercede in a storm at sea (Matthew 8:26): "And he saith to them, Why are ye fearful, O ye of little faith? Then he arose and rebuked the winds and the sea; and there was a great calm."

Over the past century, since Tennessee's Butler Act ban on teaching of evolution as scientific theory was ruled unconstitutional, the effort to inject some other form of Christian belief into science instruction has continued.

It has been a contest of separation of church and state, almost unique to the United States and its Constitution.

Those involved in the church should see it as a separation of faith and scientific theory. To force Christian belief into a science curriculum is to reduce Christianity to a scientific theory that has not been proved.

Intelligent designers claim the theory has been proved, although their hypothesis and proof are a self-fulfilling circular argument. It assumes that complex systems must be designed. The universe is complex, ergo, there must be a designer of the universe.

A counter hypothesis would be that a complex system is a series of anomalies that evolve into patterns occurring as a matter of chance. The universe is a complex system made up of anomalies that have evolved into patterns. Therefore the universe is a matter of chance.

Physicists and cosmologists conduct observations and experiments to test the validity of assumptions about the formation of the universe. To the extent that the evidence of quantum mechanics doesn't align with predictions, even Einstein's general theory of relativity remains theoretical. But a flaw in one theory doesn't prove the validity of another, assuming the Christian God (who also happens to be the Jewish and Islamic God of Abraham) is only theoretical.

Religious belief and science evolved from the same element in the human psyche that needs to explain what we are and what is happening in the world we see. Long before Abraham, tribal shaman were creating versions of gods to explain behavior of plants, animals, Earth's atmosphere, sun, moon and the stars. Forecasts of natural phenomenon were based on observations and those who were more observant of natural cycles were more successful in guiding their tribes.

That is still how science works, even as the technology for observing and analyzing natural phenomena have grown to a high level of sophistication.

It is not how religion works. Faith is a sense of human spirituality that does not rely wholly on empirical observations. It relies on a cognitive element not evident in other animals, but one that is biologically based, according to Marc Hauser, Harvard professor of psychology and biological anthropology ("Moral Minds: How nature designed our universal sense of right and wrong," HarperCollins, 2006).

Hauser says a human's moral sense results from a human's ability "to foresee future rewards" in making decisions about how to behave toward another human being. Religious beliefs are not a deciding factor in moral behavior, Hauser said. Rather, he said, moral decisions are based on the ability of the person to forecast an outcome.

Religion and science also forecast outcomes, but one relies on faith, the other on testable concepts.

University of Chicago ecology professor Jerry Coyne cites elements of scientific inquiry include having testable ideas and relying on evidence in testing a theory (www.edge.org "Must we always cater to the faithful when teaching science?")

The presence of God is not a testable idea, unless the faithful accept that God is only a theory.

Proponents of intelligent design appear to be fearful that individuals cannot exercise faith while they engage in scientific study. Matthew 8:26 offers: "Why are ye fearful, Oh ye of little faith?"

* Edwin Tanji is a former city editor of The Maui News. He can be reached at [email protected]. "Haku Mo'olelo," "writing stories," is about stories that are being written or have been written. It appears every Friday.

Pages